Results

Response at the fundamental frequency included all the responses measured at the first harmonic, but the converse was not always true. When multiplication by the cosine-bell window was included as a step in the processing, harmonic responses mimicked responses at the fundamental frequency. When the window was omitted, however, harmonic responses were less robust, and comparisons did not always attain significance in intervals corresponding to periods of significance at the fundamental frequency. Because the harmonic data added nothing to the data obtained at the fundamental frequency, only the latter are reported here.

The most consistent electrophysiological responses occurred at the two lateral derivations (figure 2). Note the difference between these data and conventional ERP recordings. The graphs are not simply the averaged signal versus time. Rather, the vertical deflection of each trace measures the amount of EEG power at the 8.9Hz frequency of interest. Each graph contains two traces, one (solid) representing the response to left-field targets, and the other (dotted) representing the response to right-field targets. The statistical test was applied not to either of these traces by itself but rather to the difference between the two traces.

During the first 300ms following the stimulus, the right hemisphere responded with an increase in phase-locked amplitude both to left-field targets and to right-field targets, whereas the left hemisphere responded significantly more to right-field targets than to left-field targets (sign test, p<0.04 in the latency ranges 161ms-283ms poststimulus in bin 0, 98ms-203ms poststimulus in bin 1, 66ms-87ms poststimulus in bin 2, and 45ms-87ms poststimulus in bin 3). The latency of this differential response in the left hemisphere decreased with increasing intertarget intervals. (At 308ms-318ms poststimulus, on the falling edge of this response, Friedman's test chi23=0.3, p~0.04; post hoc chi21=0, p~0 between bins 0 and 1. At 178ms poststimulus, on the rising edge, Friedman's test chi23=0.3, p~0.04; post hoc chi21=0, p~0 between bins 0 and 2 and between bins 0 and 3.) It is important to distinguish between response latencies and intertarget intervals. In this discussion, the term `latency' refers to the interval of time that separates a target and an electrophysiological response to that target, whereas the term `intertarget interval' refers to the interval of time that separates that same target from the previous target.

At longer latencies, amplitudes in each hemisphere tended to be greater in response to ipsilateral targets (that is, cues to shift attention to the contralateral visual hemifield) than to contralateral targets (that is, cues to shift attention to the ipsilateral visual hemifield), but there was an important exception to this generalisation in the right hemisphere at long intertarget intervals. In bin 0, phase-locked amplitude in the right hemisphere was greater in response to right targets than to left targets in the latency ranges 728ms-784ms, 920ms-931ms, and 1340ms-1368ms (sign test, p<0.04). In bin 1, however, this right-hemisphere effect inverted, so that phase-locked response was greater to left targets than to right targets in the latency range 805ms-836ms (sign test, p<0.04). In bins 2 and 3, the effect did not attain significance in either direction.

A similar effect manifested in the left hemisphere, but without the inversion in bin 1 and disappearance in bin 2. In bin 0, left-hemisphere phase-locked amplitude in response to left-field targets exceeded response to right-field targets in the latency range 1239ms-1442ms (sign test, p<0.04). In bin 1, the same effect occurred, but in the lesser latency range 889ms-903ms (sign test, p<0.04). This progression to earlier latencies continued in bin 2, at 623ms-672ms (sign test, p<0.04), and the effect disappeared in bin 3. This progression must be viewed merely as a trend, though, since Friedman tests in these intervals did not reach significance.

Tests of non-phase-locked amplitudes revealed effects complementary to those involving phase-locked amplitudes (figure 3). Since brain oscillations whose phases do not have a constant relationship to the time of stimulus presentation tend to average to zero (assuming a uniform distribution of phases), switching the order of the amplitude computation and the averaging operation reveals information about background activity. Whereas the phase-locked amplitudes tended to become greater ipsilateral to the target (that is, contralateral to the cued hemifield), non-phase-locked amplitudes tended to diminish ipsilaterally, following a brief contralateral decrease.

At the lateral derivation in the right hemisphere, non-phase-locked activity initially decreased more in response to left-field targets than in response to right-field targets (sign test, p<0.04 at 150ms-262ms poststimulus in bin 0, 112ms-182ms in bin 1, 126ms-248ms and 399-469ms in bin 2, and 164ms-360ms in bin 3). The latency of this response reached a minimum in bin 1 and became maximal in bin 3 (Friedman's test, chi23=0.3, p=0.04). Post hoc comparisons indicated significant differences on the rising edge of this response (bins (0,2) at 136ms-147ms), and on the falling edge early (bins (0, 2), (0, 3), (1, 3) at 262ms-287ms) and late (bins (0, 3), (1, 3) at 325ms-339ms). At longer latencies, right-hemisphere non-phase-locked amplitude was more suppressed in response to right-field targets than to left-field targets (sign test, p<0.04 in the latency ranges 476ms-500ms and 1004ms-1123ms in bin 0, 1078ms-1169ms in bin 1, 913ms-973ms and 1641ms-1725ms in bin 2, and 644ms-679ms in bin 3).

At the medial derivation in the right hemisphere, this pattern was inverted, and somewhat less robust. In bin 0, no significant differences between amplitudes in response to left targets and amplitudes in response to right targets occurred until late in the epoch (sign test, p<0.04 from 1788ms to 1928ms). In bin 3, amplitude in response to right targets was less than amplitude in response to left targets both initially and later in the epoch (sign test, p<0.04 at 420ms-444ms, 581ms-647ms, 721ms-738ms, and 1652ms-1659ms). In other bins, amplitude initially was more depressed in response to right targets than in response to left targets (sign test, p<0.04 in latency ranges 87ms-108ms and 283ms-315ms in bin 1, and 119ms-157ms in bin 2). At longer latencies, amplitude was more depressed in response to left targets than in response to right targets (sign test, p<0.04 at 1788ms-1928ms in bin 0 and at 980ms-1186ms in bin 2).

At the lateral derivation in the left hemisphere, the pattern appeared complementary to that at the right-hemisphere lateral derivation. Non-phase-locked activity initially decreased more in response to right-field targets than to left-field targets (sign test, p<0.04 at 563ms-577ms in bin 0 and 493ms-546ms in bin 3, with a trend at p=0.15 at 329ms-364ms in bin 1). At longer latencies, non-phase-locked amplitude decreased more in response to left-field targets than to right-field targets (sign test, p<0.04 at 1883ms-1935ms in bin 0, 1379ms-1498ms in bin 2, and 861ms-871ms and 1179ms-1232ms in bin 3).

At the medial derivation, the pattern was again inverted. Amplitude initially was more depressed in response to left targets than in response to right targets (sign test, p<0.04 at 623ms-626ms in bin 0, 164ms-241ms in bin 2, and 164ms-192ms in bin 3). At long latencies amplitude was more depressed in response to right targets than in response to left targets (sign test, p<0.04 at 752ms-787ms in bin 0, 654ms-731ms in bin 2, and 1746ms-1778ms in bin 3).

These results are complicated by the occurrence of small but systematic deviations in eye position toward the attended locations (figure 4). Amplitude changes in the horizontal electrooculogram indicate that eye movements were on the order of one degree, nowhere near the distance between the targets, but perhaps enough to influence the SSVEP. In a task that requires rapid, consecutive shifts of attention, it seems, it is impossible to inhibit saccades completely.

In the behavioural data, there was a highly significant effect of inter-target interval on the ratio of correct target detections to total number of detection opportunities (F(6, 66) = 43.27, p < 0.0001, figure 6). Detection ratios increased rapidly through the lowest inter-target intervals, and peaked in the neighbourhood of 1500ms. Response latencies decreased correspondingly (F(6, 66) = 3.49, p = 0.0047, figure 7).

Discussion